Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Fine-tuned intruder discrimination favors ant parasitoidism

  • Gabriela Pérez-Lachaud ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    igperez@ecosur.mx

    Affiliation Conservación de la Biodiversidad, El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico

  • Franklin H. Rocha,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Conservación de la Biodiversidad, El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico

  • Javier Valle-Mora,

    Roles Formal analysis, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Tapachula, Chiapas, Mexico

  • Yann Hénaut,

    Roles Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Conservación de la Biodiversidad, El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico

  • Jean-Paul Lachaud

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Conservación de la Biodiversidad, El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale (CRCA), Centre de Biologie Intégrative (CBI), Université de Toulouse; CNRS, UPS, Toulouse, France

Abstract

A diversity of arthropods (myrmecophiles) thrives within ant nests, many of them unmolested though some, such as the specialized Eucharitidae parasitoids, may cause direct damage to their hosts. Ants are known to discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates, but whether they recognize the strength of a threat and their capacity to adjust their behavior accordingly have not been fully explored. We aimed to determine whether Ectatomma tuberculatum ants exhibited specific behavioral responses to potential or actual intruders posing different threats to the host colony and to contribute to an understanding of complex ant-eucharitid interactions. Behavioral responses differed significantly according to intruder type. Ants evicted intruders that represented a threat to the colony’s health (dead ants) or were not suitable as prey items (filter paper, eucharitid parasitoid wasps, non myrmecophilous adult weevils), but killed potential prey (weevil larvae, termites). The timing of detection was in accordance with the nature and size of the intruder: corpses (a potential source of contamination) were detected faster than any other intruder and transported to the refuse piles within 15 min. The structure and complexity of behavioral sequences differed among those intruders that were discarded. Workers not only recognized and discriminated between several distinct intruders but also adjusted their behavior to the type of intruder encountered. Our results confirm the previously documented recognition capabilities of E. tuberculatum workers and reveal a very fine-tuned intruder discrimination response. Colony-level prophylactic and hygienic behavioral responses through effective removal of inedible intruders appears to be the most general and flexible form of defense in ants against a diverse array of intruders. However, this generalized response to both potentially lethal and harmless intruders might have driven the evolution of ant-eucharitid interactions, opening a window for parasitoid attack and allowing adult parasitoid wasps to quickly leave the natal nest unharmed.

Introduction

Ants are among the most diverse and abundant organisms on earth. Their complex nests and colonies provide both rich, homeostatic microhabitats and available resources that are exploited by other organisms [13]. Despite the aggressive behavior and sophisticated defense strategies of most ant species, many organisms (termed in general myrmecophiles) have managed to deal with ant aggressiveness and bypass their defense strategies [2,47]. In response, ants have evolved a suite of physiological, immunological and behavioral defensive responses to counter exploitation by micro- and macro-parasites both at the individual and the colony level [813].

An efficient system to discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates is essential not only to maintain the integrity, cohesion and functioning of social insect societies [2], but also to detect intruders and effectively defend the colony [14,15]. Ants are known to effectively discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates, but their capabilities to detect a threat and adjust their behavioral responses to its intensity have only been addressed recently [1618].

Nestmate recognition and communication in social insects is based extensively on chemical signals, i.e., cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) [1921], although acoustics also play an important role [22]. Myrmecophiles range from highly integrated guests that rely on physiological, morphological and behavioral adaptations that allow them to be treated as nestmates, to poorly integrated species that elude the hosts as much as possible [23]. Studies on myrmecophiles that exploit ant communication signals to successfully integrate the society are numerous [24,25]. However, very few studies have addressed the processes influencing ant defensive response behaviors against intruders that emerge within the host nest but do not integrate into the colonies [26], which is the case of brood parasitoids and some social parasites. Individual and colony level behavioral defenses represent lower-cost defenses than their physiological counterparts [27], so it is expected that these low-cost defenses be first used when ants encounter an uninvited guest. For example, in bees, only a reduced investment is necessary to express nest sanitation behavior which effectively prevents parasite establishment [28].

Defense at the colony level, also termed “social immunity” [11], encompasses all behaviors that help to prevent invasion by micro- and macro-parasites or infection spread, and includes characteristic sanitation behaviors such as elimination of diseased brood, weeding of infected fungus in leaf-cutter ants, allo-grooming, undertaking (removal of diseased/dead adults; also termed necrophoresis), and waste management among others [11,12,28]. Expressing these behaviors has been shown to effectively impact on the survival and demography of the colony [29]. This is the case particularly with regard to pathogens since social insects are vulnerable to disease transmission due to high density, interaction rate and relatedness of individuals within colonies [5,8,18]. However, how social insects, and in particular ants, combat macro-parasites (arthropods) that threaten their colony resources or their brood has not been fully addressed. It has been suggested that once an alien is inside the nest or emerges within the nest, colony-level prophylactic and hygienic behavioral responses such as active removal of intruders are probably the most general and flexible forms of defense to eliminate them [15,30]. Paradoxically, this general response (removal), well suited to cope with disease [29], might have created a window of evolutionary opportunity for ant parasitoids and might have driven the evolution of ant-parasitoid interactions.

Ants are hosts to at least 17 orders of arthropod myrmecophiles [2,4] spanning from generalist scavengers in the nest (posing no threat to ants and even being beneficial), to very specialized, specific parasitoids and brood predators, which cause direct damage to the colony. Ant parasitoids include representatives of several wasp, fly and mite families [3133]. Of these, the wasp family Eucharitidae (Hymenoptera) stands out since it is the only family of insects known to exclusively parasitize ants [34,35]. These specific ant parasitoids have a very specialized life cycle [31,34,36]. Females oviposit away from the host upon vegetation; the active searching first instar larvae access the colony through phoresis on foraging ants and develop as ectoparasitoids, initially attaching to ant larvae but completing development when the host pupates [37,38]. Adults emerge inside the host nest among the workers and must escape from the natal nest to mate and reproduce [34,36,38]. Coevolution between ants and these specialized parasitoids is intriguing. It has long been known that ants, in general, do not treat newly emerged eucharitid adults aggressively [3944], which suggests that both immature and adults may exploit ant communication signals while they are in the nest [42,44]. In a previous study, we experimentally characterized the interactions between freshly eclosed eucharitid parasitoid wasps (Dilocantha lachaudii Heraty) and their Ectatomma tuberculatum (Olivier) hosts and analyzed their chemical profiles [15]. The results showed that, although partially mimicking the chemical profile of their ant hosts, freshly emerged parasitoids were immediately recognized as alien and removed from the nest [15]. Unscathed wasps were disposed by ants in waste piles outside the nest and could disperse, suggesting that rapid removal of eucharitids by their hosts actually increases the opportunity for these short-lived wasps to complete their life cycle. We also hypothesized that the ant’s general nest hygiene behavior may well be exploited by eucharitids and other ant parasitoids, allowing them to quickly leave the natal nest [15]. However, this previous study left open several questions concerning the specificity of the response of ants: a) whether the observed behavior was specific to eucharitid parasitoids or was a generalized response to any intruder independent of the degree of threat to the colony (i.e., a true general defensive strategy of ants); b) whether removal of eucharitids was comparable to the necrophoric behavior commonly exhibited by ants [1,45,46] as suggested in earlier studies [41]; or c) whether the relatively low aggressiveness of ants was mediated by the calm behavior of the eucharitids (wasps behaving in a steady way, most of the time freezing at contact with an ant), as suggested for other myrmecophiles [47].

In the present study, we carried out an experiment to determine whether ants exhibit differential behavioral responses to intruders that vary in intrinsic characteristics or in the degree of threat to the colony. We aimed at answering the following questions: 1) Do E. tuberculatum ants remove any alien organism or object from their nests? 2) Do they discriminate among distinct aliens? We further contrasted our results with the behavioral response of ants when encountering eucharitid parasitoid wasps, in an effort to try to disentangle interactions between ants and their specialized parasitoids.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

This study was non-invasive and complied with Mexican law. Collection of insect specimens was authorized by SEMARNAT/DGVS (Permit number Faut-0277 to GP-L). The collection did not involve endangered or protected species. The sites where we collected our species were not protected in any way. Only necessary numbers of E. tuberculatum were collected to conduct the experiments.

Study organisms

Ectatomma tuberculatum (Formicidae: Ectatomminae) is a Neotropical, generalist, predatory ant, with hypogeic nests built at the base of trees or shrubs. The nest entrance is extended by a chimney made of soil and fragments of vegetable matter [48], which is permanently guarded by specialized workers. Monogyny or facultative polygyny have been reported for different Mexican populations [49,50]. Mature colonies have monomorphic workers with little variation in size [51], but with a well-defined age polyethism in which young workers take care of the brood and then progressively move to foraging tasks while ageing [52,53]. Workers possess a venomous sting and behave aggressively (i.e., repeated stinging and biting) when presented with ants from another colony/species or with potential prey [54].

Colony collection and maintenance

Four mature, queenright colonies of E. tuberculatum were studied. Colonies were excavated and collected in January 2013 in Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico (18° 30' 4.54" N; 88° 19' 47.74" W) (S1 Table). The ants were reared in plastic nest boxes (23 x 17 x 10 cm) provided with a glass vial filled with water and stuffed with cotton at one end, and a dry leaf to provide concealment and darkness. Each nest box was connected to a foraging arena (a 30 x 23 x 8 cm, plastic box) via a transparent glass tube (1 cm diam. x 30 cm length). The ants were fed sliced apple, diluted honey and Tenebrio molitor L. larvae (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae) twice a week. Mealworm supply was stopped 3 days prior to and during bioassays. Ants were maintained under laboratory conditions (25–26°C and 50–70% RH and under natural photoperiod) for 4 months before experiments.

Bioassays

Laboratory tests were conducted using artificially reduced colonies, homogeneous in size with one queen, 50 workers chosen at random from the initial colony (from both the nest and the foraging arena), and the brood. Reduction in colony size does not affect colony functioning [30]. Ant behavior against alien organisms or items inside their nests (hereafter intruders, Table 1) was studied by experimentally placing an intruder in the middle of the nest box. Interactions were video recorded for 15 min beginning immediately after the introduction of the intruder. Observations were performed during May-August 2013, eight to 12 trials were performed per day, between 07:00 and 15:00 h. Treatments were allocated at random to the different colonies (completely randomization design blocked on colony); trials with the same colony were separated by at least 30 min. Fifteen trials per treatment and per colony were conducted for a total of 60 trials per treatment. Intruders were used only once.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of characteristics of intruders used in this study.

Ants were presented with a series of organisms/items providing different multichannel cues that could reveal the stimuli that triggered their behavioral response when encountering an eucharitid eclosing in the nest. These treatments further allowed to test some of the hypotheses that have been put forward to explain ejection of wasps from the natal nest without escalated aggression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.t001

Treatments

We analyzed ant behavior using a series of objects and live or dead insects (Table 1). Intruders were chosen to investigate four different characteristics that possibly trigger ant responses: mobility, size, whether they are edible, and differences in chemical profile, although these traits cannot be completely disentangled when working with live insects. Six items were tested: (i) motile potential prey (Nasutitermes sp. termite workers); (ii) non-motile potential prey (Caulophilus oryzae (Gyllenhal) weevil larvae); (iii) live, inedible, small beetles (adult C. oryzae weevils) as a proxy of live eucharitids due to the possibility of triggering ant behavior through movement detection; (iv) dead, conspecific workers which represent a threat to the colony health; (v) pentane-washed eucharitid wasps, as a proxy of morphological/structural cues provided by eucharitids but in the absence of both chemical cues (no odor or, at least, very reduced signals) and movement; and (vi) filter paper balls as a control. Ectatomma ants are known to rely predominantly on visual cues for orientation and foraging [6063], and are very reactive to movement during predation [51,64]. Consequently, motile or large sized intruders were expected to generate a more rapid response than motionless or smaller ones. Similarly, we hypothesized that dead conspecific ants, representing a threat to the colony’s health, would elicit a more extreme behavioral response from ants than filter paper balls, for example.

Ectatomma tuberculatum colonies are parasitized by eucharitids all year round, but generally parasitism rates are very low [50]. Due to the constraints imposed by our experimental design (random allocation of intruders) and because of limited wasp availability, we used recordings from a previous study to characterize behavioral interactions between recently emerged (0–1 day-old) live parasitoids and ants [15]. Recordings were de novo visualized and analyzed to produce a flow diagram (see below) which was contrasted against the behavior of ants when confronted with the different intruders tested in this study; such a flow diagram for ants interacting with eucharitids had not been generated before. The bioassays of both our previous and present studies were performed in a similar manner and under similar conditions, but differed in the duration of trials and in the number of workers present in the nests (interactions with live wasps lasted at most 10 min) [15].

Termite workers were collected in the surroundings of Chetumal. They are a common prey for E. tuberculatum and may constitute up to 9% of their prey in natural field conditions [65]. Larvae and adults of the broad-nosed granary weevil C. oryzae (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) are currently reared in our laboratory. The hard cuticle of weevil adults might prevent ant attack; furthermore, they are similar in size to eucharitids [30]. Alcohol preserved adult eucharitid wasps (D. lachaudii) were air dried, soaked in 2 ml GC-grade n-pentane (Fluka Analytical, 99%) for 5 min, and air dried again before use in the bioassays. Conspecific, homo-colonial workers were killed by freezing and then were maintained at room temperature for 48h before trials. Freshly killed Myrmica rubra (L.) ants rarely elicit necrophoric behavior in their nestmates while 100% of corpses two-days post mortem do [59]. Filter paper balls were hand-made using pieces of 1 cm2 Whatman filter paper and wearing surgical gloves to avoid contamination during manipulation.

Obtaining data from recordings

Video recordings were independently visualized by two observers. We evaluated the following parameters: latency (the time elapsed between the introduction of the intruder and when the ants initiated a contact), handling plus transport time, and the outcome of interactions. The outcome of the behavioral interactions was scored as: (i) “discarded” (the intruder was removed from the nest or thrown on the interior refusal pile that E. tuberculatum colonies usually maintain); (ii) “brood provision” (the intruder was stung and injured or killed, then transported to the dry leaf concealing the brood where it was preyed upon; (iii) “ignored” (workers came into contact with the intruder, antennated it without picking it up or transporting it); and (iv) “no decision yet” when ants had not made a clear decision at the end of the observation period (a worker was holding the intruder between its mandibles, and remained still in the middle of the nest). We drew flow diagrams for each intruder type and compared them to the flow diagram obtained using previous recordings of interactions of ants with live wasps. In these figures, 1st order Markov matrices for transition frequencies between two successive behaviors were calculated. Because several ants could participate in interactions with intruders, and the same ant could encounter an intruder more than once during a trial, any trial could include several repetitive short behavioral sequences (loops) ending in item abandon, a behavior referred to as ‘giving- up’ (see below). For each intruder type, percentages were calculated based on the overall number of transitions between individual behavioral acts. All behavioral transitions were represented in the flow diagrams. Primary transitions (those that appeared in at least 3% of the sequences) defined the typical behavioral sequence for a given intruder type.

Statistical analyses

The outcome of interactions, i.e., the behavioral decision of ants, was analyzed using a logistic regression model with a multinomial response [66,67]. Proportion of intruders that were discarded were compared with Fisher’s exact tests. To determine whether latency and handling plus transport time varied according to the intruder type we used a Generalized Linear Mixed-effects Model (GLMM), with “colony” as a random factor and a binomial error structure fitted to the natural log transformed data [68]. Analyses were performed in R [69]. Data represent mean ± SEM.

Results

Outcome of behavioral interactions

A total of 360 trials were conducted. Seven trials were excluded from the analyses: 5 recordings were damaged and on two occasions ants did not come into contact with the intruder during the trials. Ants detected the intruder in all 353 remaining observations; of these the intruder was ignored in 35 trials (9.9%), predominantly those with filter paper balls (18 cases) and pentane washed eucharitids (11 cases; Fig 1). In 17 trials (4.8%), the observation period ended while a worker was holding the intruder between its mandibles and no clear decision could be assessed (termites: 4 cases; adult weevils: 9 cases; weevil larvae: 4 cases). Trials with a clear behavioral decision from ants amounted 301. Globally, 89.7% (105/117) of potential prey were effectively preyed upon while 79.7% (188/236) of inedible intruders were discarded during the 15 min observation period.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Behavioral response of Ectatomma tuberculatum ants against a diverse array of intruders experimentally introduced into their nests.

Output of interactions were scored as: “discarded” (the intruder was removed from the nest or deposited in the inner refuse pile in the nest); “brood provisioning” (the intruder was captured and given to the brood as food); “ignored” (the intruder was antennated briefly but remained in its original location at the end of the trial); or “no decision” (no behavioral decision made, the intruder was held between the mandibles of a worker at the end of the trial). Four colonies were tested, n = 60 replicates per treatment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.g001

Ants behavioral response against intruders differed significantly according to the intruder type (logistic regression, χ2treatments = 411.0, d.f. = 15, p < 0.0001; Fig 1). There were differences among the colonies studied (χ2colonies = 37.6, d.f. = 9, p < 0.0001), but there was no interaction between these two factors (χ2treatments x colonies = 35.26, d.f. = 45, p = 0.85). The very efficient nest hygiene response of Colony #1, which did not ignore any intruder, accounted for the differences among colonies (S1 Fig).

Almost all dead conspecific ants were transported to the refuse piles within the 15 min of observation (96.5%, 56/58; 12 to the refuse pile inside the nest and 44 outside). Similarly, pentane-washed wasps, adult weevils and filter paper pieces were, in the vast majority, discarded from the nest (in 81.4, 71.7 and 69.5% of the respective trials). There were no differences among these treatments (Fig 1). By contrast, 91.2% of C. oryzae larvae and 88.3% of termites were provided to the brood as food (Fig 1). These two last treatments (termites and weevil larvae) were statistically different from the other treatments (95% confidence intervals, S2 Fig). In fact, only the latter two types of intruders (potential prey) were killed or injured; intruders from all other treatments in this study were never injured, and both filter paper balls and corpses did not elicit any aggressive behavior.

The site where ants discarded intruders (refuse pile inside the nest box or outside the nest) was related to intruder type (Pearson´s test of independence, χ2 = 9.76, d.f. = 3, p < 0.05; Table 2). Filter paper balls were significantly more frequently abandoned in the internal refuse pile than dead ants (Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.044, S2 Table) or adult weevils (Fisher exact test, p = 0.015) which were, in most cases, immediately removed from the nest; pentane-washed wasps were equally transported to both the refuse pile in the nest or outside, and proportions differed only from those of adult weevils. However, all intruders deposited in the interior refuse pile during the observation time were finally removed from the nest one or two hours later.

thumbnail
Table 2. Site of removal and percent of individuals discarded by Ectatomma tuberculatum workers in 15 min trials.

Data from 4 colonies. N is the number of trials in which intruders were discarded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.t002

Latency and time for handling and transport

Latency varied according to intruder type (GLMM, χ2 = 131.46, d.f. = 5, p < 0.001). Dead conspecific workers and filter paper balls were contacted by workers significantly faster (0.50 ± 0.03 min and 0.67 ± 0.06 min after introduction, respectively) than individuals in the other treatments (Fig 2A). Contrastingly, it took longer to detect pentane-washed eucharitids and weevil larvae (1.24 ± 0.14 min and 2.03 ± 0.24 min, respectively), and intermediate latency times characterized trials with termites and adult weevils (0.74 ± 0.06 min and 0.86 ± 0.10 min, respectively) (Fig 2A).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Latency and time for handling and transport of intruders by Ectatomma tuberculatum workers.

A) Mean (± SEM) latency to contact and B) Mean handling plus transport time. Bioassays were performed with 4 different colonies. Fifteen trials per treatment and per colony were conducted for a total of 60 trials per treatment. Different letters indicate significant differences (p < 0.05). Mean (± SEM) values for live eucharitids from a previous study [15], are shown for reference only, and were not considered in the statistical analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.g002

Handling plus transport time also varied according to intruder type (GLMM, χ2 = 103.65, d.f. = 5, p < 0.001, Fig 2B). Handling and transport of termites and both adult weevils and larvae took significantly longer (more than 5 minutes) compared to the time required to handle dead conspecific ants (3.94 ± 0.39 min), pentane-washed eucharitid wasps (2.55 ± 0.32 min) or filter paper balls (1.34 ± 0.12 min).

Ant behavioral sequences according to intruder type

Fourteen distinct behavioral acts were involved in interactions with intruders: searching, detection through contact, antennation, mandible strike, seizure-lifting, abdomen bending, stinging attempt, carrying, robbing, failure, giving-up (i.e. abandon of the item after antennation, a behavior that depicts ant motivation), removal, holding motionless and brood provisioning. “Holding motionless” is a behavioral state and refers to a predator subduing a prey and holding it between its mandibles while staying immobile. Cook [48] noticed that when an E. tuberculatum worker seized a termite, it “held it in its jaws for fully five minutes”. Similarly, workers of this species may remain immobile while holding honey between their mandibles in a behavior known as resource storage [51]. For a detailed description of the rest of the behaviors see [30].

The structure and complexity of behavioral sequences differed according to the intruder type. Simple sequences (low number of behavioral acts and few main transitions) were observed with static intruders (ant corpses, pentane-washed wasps and filter paper balls). The behavioral sequence with the simplest (yet the most direct) structure was elicited by corpses of conspecific ants (nine behavioral acts, six major behavioral transitions; Fig 3, Table 3). Corpses elicited high interest and were thoroughly investigated by ants which came repeatedly into contact with them and then departed (20 ± 1.5 contacts per trial; Table 3), promoting numerous new short searching sequences, each one ending in giving-up; however, in 96.5% of the 58 trials the corpse was discarded.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers confronted with dead conspecific adult ants.

A total of 1168 short incomplete sequences ending in giving-up by the ant, and 56 sequences ending in the removal of the intruder were analyzed (in total: 1224 sequences). The percentage values were calculated as the observed transition frequencies between two successive behavioral acts, divided by the number of sequences. The thickness of each arrow is proportional to the percent value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of characteristics of the behavioral sequences of E. tuberculatum workers confronted with different types of intruders.

Data from 4 colonies; 15 trials per intruder and colony, with the exception of live wasps for which 30 observations were obtained from a previous study [15], but were analyzed here.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.t003

Trials with filter paper balls and pentane-washed parasitoids involved 10 and 12 behavioral acts, respectively, and few major transitions (Table 3, S3 and S4 Figs). These two types of intruders were mostly gently lifted; however, on a few occasions, and unlike dead ants and filter paper balls, pentane-washed parasitoids were the object of a few mandible strikes and stinging attempts; robbing events between workers also occurred during transport (S4 Fig).

Contrastingly, live intruders prompted the expression of the whole defensive/predatory repertoire of ants: behavioral sequences were complex and involved 12 behaviors and between 18 to 21 main transitions (Table 3, Fig 4 and S5S7 Figs). Failure to catch a live intruder promoted intensive searching once more. Mandibular strike, the sudden closure of mandibles upon the intruder at first contact, was very frequent with live eucharitids (87% of the sequences, Fig 4) and adult weevils (50.7% of the sequences, S5 Fig) while soft-bodied termites and larvae were frequently thoroughly antennated before lifting (S6 and S7 Figs). Stinging attempts (S8 Fig) were provoked by all types of live intruders; however, stinging was only successful with soft bodied termites and weevil larvae in this study. Robbing (a worker fiercely taking the intruder from the transporting worker) was noted with termites (S8 Fig), weevil larvae and live eucharitids, but not with adult weevils (ants did not struggle for transporting weevils). Ants appeared to have some difficulty grabbing adult weevils as, in most trials, ants failed to capture this intruder at the first mandibular strike. Trials with live intruders resulted in high levels of removal and disposal into refuse piles in the cases of live adult weevils (71.7% of the 60 trials) and live wasps (93.3% of the 30 trials, [15]) or in high levels of brood provisioning in the cases of live termites (88.3% of the 60 trials) and live weevil larvae (91.2% of the 57 trials).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers faced with live eucharitid parasitoids.

The flow diagram represents a total of 26 incomplete sequences ending in the giving-up by the ant, and 28 complete sequences ending in the removal of the intruder (in total: 54 sequences). Recordings from a previous study [15] were analyzed de novo.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.g004

Several ants were involved in trials with live insects. Termites were contacted a mean of 13.8 ± 0.9 times, adult weevils and larvae a mean of 4.7 ± 0.3 and 3.5 ± 0.2 times respectively (Table 3), while live parasitoids were, in general, removed by the first or second ant that came into contact with them and were contacted on average 2.2 ± 0.3 times [15]. Workers displayed a frenzied behavior when transporting wasps, which froze and remained still while being transported [15].

Discussion

Recognition is a basic, major component of biological systems, allowing the distinction of self from non-self and the identification of different classes of non-self [70], and is particularly important in the context of prey/predator discrimination. Like most other social insects, workers of E. tuberculatum discriminate between nestmates and non-nestmates [54]; they are also able to discriminate their sibling species, the inquiline social parasite E. parasiticum Feitosa & Fresneau, from their conspecifics [71]. In this study we experimentally demonstrated that E. tuberculatum workers were, in addition, able to recognize and discriminate among several distinct potential or actual intruders and that they further adjusted their behavior to the type of intruder encountered in the nest. As expected, workers readily discriminated between potential prey and other types of intruders (almost 90% of potential prey were effectively preyed on during the 15 min observation period). This contrasted with the quick removal of the rest of intruders tested here, including those that represented a sanitary risk (dead ants) as well as filter paper, pentane-washed parasitoid wasps, and adult weevils, i.e., all intruders that apparently were of no value as edible items. Similarly, in a previous study [15], inedible live eucharitid parasitoids were quickly removed from the nest. Although there was a high variance in response between the colony fragments tested here, the proportion and type of intruders that were removed/predated were consistent across the four colonies tested. Inter-individual and/or colony personality may explain the observed variability [72,73]. A remarkable consistency in the response of ants was similarly observed in a previous study [15], that included observations on several colonies collected in different years.

Although several different types of intruders elicited the same behavioral response, i.e., removal from the nest, suggesting a general solution to a common problem, trials with intruders that were discarded differed in manner (behavioral acts performed, number of major transitions between behaviors and structure of the behavioral sequence), timing, and location to where the distinct intruders were transported. These differences suggest the existence of high recognition and fine-tuned discrimination capabilities in E. tuberculatum. Accurate decision-making related to foraging or to a potential threat is crucial for survival, and in social insects this can be achieved either at the individual or colony levels [16,7274]. Early detection of parasites and/or intruders allows for the initiation of defense mechanisms [18] and is considered crucial to the success of the colony. Fine intruder discrimination abilities have been reported in other social insects, such as bees and stingless bees [75,76]. In Apis mellifera L., for example, empirical experimental studies have shown that different invaders elicit different defensive responses in guard bees, from antennation to escalated aggression depending on the threat to the colony [75,77]. In Temnothorax longispinosus (Roger) ants, potential enemies (a non-competitor ant species, non-nestmate conspecifics, a competitor species, and a social parasite) are treated differently, based on the threat they inflict on the colony [16]. Furthermore, differential aggressive behavior against myrmecophiles that differ in their degree of integration into the colony has been reported for some ant species such as Eciton burchellii foreli Mayr, Leptogenys processionalis distinguenda (Emery), L. borneensis Wheeler, or Formica rufa L. [23,78,79].

Our results suggest that both intrinsic characteristics of intruders and the threat posed to ants, influence ant-intruder interactions. As hypothesized, intruder detection by E. tuberculatum workers was influenced by size and movement: large intruders (ant corpses, filter paper balls) and intruders in movement (adult weevils, termites, live wasps) were detected and contacted significantly earlier than small sized or motionless intruders (pentane-washed eucharitids, weevil larvae), which is consistent with Ectatomma ants using predominantly visual cues for orientation and foraging [6063]. Notably, in our observations, escalated aggression (stinging/killing) was observed only with potential prey, but not (or very infrequently) with other live intruders which were removed unharmed from the nest. Many organisms are known to maximize energy efficiency by engaging in behaviors that are energetically conservative. Since escalated aggression and venom production are costly processes [80,81], accurate recognition of potential prey or judging of potential intruder threats by E. tuberculatum workers and subsequent adjustment of their behavior in accordance to the threat (escalated aggression or not), can help individuals to avoid wasting time and energy on costly behaviors and hence improve foraging or defense at the colony level.

The response of ants against live adult eucharitids and live adult weevils, small but mobile insects with a hard cuticle, was quite similar, and though interactions were in most cases not fatal, mandible strikes and stinging attempts were frequently employed with these two intruders, demonstrating a certain level of aggressiveness. Sequences with weevils and eucharitids differed though in the interest that these two intruders elicited in ants and the eagerness of ants to get rid of eucharitids (frenzied behavior of ants and robbing of the intruder between nestmates was observed with live eucharitids but not with adult weevils), which might be explained by differences in their CHC profiles. By contrast, the response of ants to pentane-washed wasps (likely bearing few or no CHCs) was characterized by prolonged antennation of the static wasp and the virtual absence of aggressive behaviors. Absence of or reduced aggression of ants against non-nestmates and some myrmecophiles is thought to correlate with a reduction in chemical cues (chemical insignificance hypothesis; [58]). This hypothesis might explain the low aggressiveness of ants against live eucharitids but under closer examination does not explain their removal from the nest. Furthermore, it appears that intruder recognition in E. tuberculatum does not rely exclusively on chemical cues because items such as pentane-washed wasps and filter paper balls were also removed from the nest. Significantly, the response of ants with pentane-washed wasps, filter paper balls, and dead conspecific ants was characterized by prolonged antennation and the gentle seizure of the intruder. These three types of immobile intruders bearing distinct chemical cues triggered a thorough exploration in the ants but no aggression, suggesting a role for movement in eliciting aggression. Motion, supplemented perhaps with chemical cues, may release aggressive behaviors such as mandible strike and stinging attempts and may promote the willingness of ants to remove live wasps.

Taken together, our observations suggest that inedible intruders are systematically removed from the nest, no matter their nature, and indicate the combination of at least three intruder traits affecting the expression of individual ant behavior during interactions which could explain the relatively low aggressive behavior of ants against the parasitoid wasps: a) morphological/structural defenses of eucharitids and other similar intruders (tough cuticle) that hampers ant attack, b) thanatosis and calm behavior or immobility of the intruder, and c) tiny size of the intruder relative to ants (see Table 1 and S8 Fig). In trials with adult weevils, stinging attempts were numerous but unsuccessful; the morphology and hard cuticle of weevils were apparently an impediment to achieve attack. Likewise, stinging attempts were in most cases unsuccessful with live eucharitids, and escalated attacks (sustained stinging attempts until wasp death) have been observed only twice as part of another study [15].

Members of Eucharitidae are all ant parasitoids and their interaction with ants is shaped by a long co-evolutionary history, with eucharitids colonizing ants approximately 72 My ago [35]. Members of the tribe Eucharitini exhibit an extraordinary amount of morphological variation, particularly concerning the thoracic spines (from cylindrical to carapace-like [35], see also Fig. 26 in [82] and Fig 6 in [83]) that usually cover the gaster, the part of the body most susceptible to physical harm (GP-L, pers. obs.). A protective role of these posterior scutellar processes during interactions with aggressive ants has already been suggested [82], and our results with eucharitids and adult weevils add further support for such a protective role of both the hard cuticle of some insects and the thoracic spines of Eucharitini. Analogous structural defenses have been reported in various other myrmecophiles that exhibit different life styles. For example, a hard exoskeleton allows the brood predator beetle Myrmecaphodius excavaticollis (Blanchard) to withstand initial interactions with Solenopsis invicta Buren workers before acquiring the host colony odor [84]; while the hard but smooth cuticle of the adults of the mesostigmatid parasitoid mite Macrodynichus sellnicki Hirschmann & Zirngiebl-Nicol thwarts Nylanderia fulva (Mayr) ants from grasping them, allowing the mites to remain in the nest and attack the brood [85].

As already noted, and as for several other ant species [8689], our results suggest an important role of movement in triggering stinging in E. tuberculatum workers. This indicates that the freezing and calm behavior of live eucharitids is responsible for the relatively low aggressiveness frequently reported in the literature in ant-eucharitid interactions [3944], and invoked in other myrmecophiles to explain ant tolerance [47]. Static, inedible intruders in this study (pentane-washed wasps, dead ants and filter paper balls) were not treated aggressively, though ants did discriminate among them. Significantly, dead ants elicited a stronger response than the other two static intruders (many more ants came into contact with dead ants that were antennated more frequently than filter paper balls or pentane-washed wasps) and elicited the most direct (in terms of sequence structure) and fastest response in ants. This is consistent with the rapid disposal of corpses documented for other ant species. It is known that oleic acid, a compound produced by the decomposition of corpses, is a strong stimulus that releases necrophoric behavior in ants [29,90]. Necrophoresis and refuse disposal have been previously advanced as plausible explanations of eucharitid removal from the host nest [15,41]. However, a detailed comparison of the flow diagrams between live eucharitids and corpses (Figs 3 and 4) did not support the necrophoresis hypothesis as an explanation of live eucharitid transport and removal from the natal nest.

Incidentally, the relative small size of eucharitids, reinforced by their calm behavior, may contribute to reduced ant aggressiveness. Adult wasps are small relative to Ectatomma workers (S8 Fig) and though they are recognized as alien, they might not be perceived as a real threat [15]. Similarly, low aggression of the host was also noted for two species of Macrodinychus mites which are only 2 mm in length and therefore small when compared with their Leptogenys hosts: in artificial nests, mites were frequently picked up and dumped at the nest refuse site [26].

Our final aim was to try to elucidate the stimuli that promote wasp removal without aggression. On the whole, E. tuberculatum workers removed most of the intruders from their nests, with the exclusion of potential prey, illustrating a very efficient nest hygiene behavior which may well have had consequences for the evolution of ant parasitoidism. Very few studies have examined the interactions with intruders that develop inside the nest during a stage that is “invisible” to ants before emerging and attempting to leave the natal site to continue with their life cycle (e.g. offspring of social parasites and parasitoids that emerge inside the host colonies). It is known, for example, that male offspring of the bumblebee social parasite, Bombus vestalis vestalis (Geoffroy), which actively produces sexual pheromones, uses repellent substances to escape from host aggression during their short intranidal life [14]. However, to our knowledge, eucharitid parasitoids do not employ such a strategy and no appeasement substances have been reported in these wasps [42]. Rapid escape from the natal nest is important to the wasps’ overall fitness because they are very short-lived, and exploitation of the predictable hygiene nest response of ants (systematic removal) by eucharitids, represents a quick means of leaving the host nest [15]. The generalization of the removal response to adult weevils in this study seems to support the exploitation of the ant hygienic response hypothesis by D. lachaudii, and also by a number of other ant parasitoids.

Despite the range of potential or actual intruders used in our experiment, E. tuberculatum workers behaved in a very stereotyped and specific way according to the type of intruder encountered within their nests, exhibiting a very flexible response which is congruent with both their very flexible behavior during foraging [62] and their learning capabilities [63, 91]. Our results showed an adjustment of their behavior to the characteristics of the intruder, with a heightened defensive response towards parasitoids and adult weevils and a rapid removal of corpses, but no aggression toward invaders that did not represent a threat to the colony. This behavioral flexibility may have an adaptive value in the field when ants have to deal with a variety of natural enemies such as parasitoids or conspecific and heterospecific alien ants that intrude into the nests to steal food, for example [9294]. From an adaptive perspective, the more generalized but flexible the defensive response, the more readily the colonies adjust to potential threats.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Colony related effect upon the behavioral response of Ectatomma tuberculatum ants against a diverse array of intruders experimentally introduced into their nests.

Colony 1 showed the most efficient nest hygienic behavior: the probability of an intruder being ignored was null.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Effect of treatment (intruder type) upon the behavioral response of Ectatomma tuberculatum ants.

Termites and weevil larvae treatments differed from the rest of the treatments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers with filter paper balls.

A total of 575 short incomplete sequences and 42 sequences ending in the removal of the filter paper were analyzed (in total: 617 sequences).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s003

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers faced with dead, pentane-washed, eucharitid parasitoids.

In most trials initial inspection of the intruder ended rapidly after a brief contact with the wasp. A total of 213 incomplete and 49 complete sequences were analyzed (in total: 262 sequences).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s004

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers faced with live adult Caulophilus oryzae weevils.

A total of 222 incomplete, 37 complete and 17 sequences that ended with the weevil being held by a worker were analyzed (in total: 276 sequences).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s005

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Behavioral interactions of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers with live termites.

A total of 729 incomplete sequences ending in the giving-up of the ant, 53 complete sequences ending with the termite being provided to the brood, and 4 sequences ending with the termite held by a worker were analyzed (in total: 786 sequences).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s006

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers faced with live Caulophilus oryzae larvae.

A total of 157 incomplete sequences, 52 complete sequences ending with the intruder transported to the brood, and 4 sequences ending with the intruder being held between the mandibles of a worker were analyzed (in total: 213 sequences).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s007

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Behavior of Ectatomma tuberculatum workers during interactions with live intruders.

A) Stinging attempt against an adult weevil; B) Robbing (a worker fiercely taking a termite transported by a nestmate); C) Transport of a live eucharitid; D) Transport of a live adult weevil. Background of A, B and D is 1 mm square graph paper.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s008

(TIF)

S1 Table. Composition of Ectatomma tuberculatum colonies.

Colonies were excavated and collected in January 2013 in Chetumal, Quintana Roo, Mexico (18° 30' 4.54" N; 88° 19' 47.74" W).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s009

(TIF)

S2 Table. Pairwise comparisons of proportion of intruders directly removed from the nest and those transported to the interior refuse pile.

Fisher’s exact test probabilities; significantly different values in bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s010

(TIF)

S3 Table. Latency, handling and transport time, outcome of interactions, and size of intruders and E. tuberculatum workers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210739.s011

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Humberto Bahena Basave (ECOSUR) for help with the preparation of some figures. We are particularly grateful to two anonymous reviewers for their valuable and helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Wheeler WM. Ants, their structure, development and behavior. New York, USA: Mac Millan, The Columbia University Press; 1910.
  2. 2. Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The Ants. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University Press; 1990.
  3. 3. Hughes DP, Pierce NE, Boomsma JJ. Social insect symbionts: evolution in homeostatic fortresses. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008; 23(12): 672–677. pmid:18951653
  4. 4. Kistner DH. The social insects’ bestiary. In: Hermann HR, editor. Social Insects. New York, USA: Academic Press; 1982. pp. 1–244.
  5. 5. Schmid-Hempel P. Parasites in Social Insects. Princeton, USA: Princeton University Press; 1998.
  6. 6. Lachaud J-P, Lenoir A, Witte V (eds.). Ants and their parasites. Psyche, Special Issue. New York, USA: Hindawi Publishing Corporation; 2012. https://www.hindawi.com/journals/psyche/si/726548/.
  7. 7. Lachaud J-P, Lenoir A, Hughes DP (eds.). Ants and their parasites 2013. Psyche Special Issue. New York, USA: Hindawi Publishing Corporation; 2013. https://www.hindawi.com/journals/psyche/si/420253/.
  8. 8. Boomsma JJ, Schmid-Hempel P, Hughes WOH. Life histories and parasite pressure across the major groups of social insects. In: Fellowes MDE, Holloway GJ, Rolff J, editors. Insect evolutionary ecology. Proceeding of the Royal Entomological Society’s 22nd Symposium. Wallingford, U.K.: CABI Publishing; 2005. pp. 139–175.
  9. 9. Cremer S, Sixt M. Analogies in the evolution of individual and social immunity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009; 364(1513): 129–142. pmid:18926974
  10. 10. Hughes DP. The extended phenotype within the colony and how it obscures social communication. In: d’Ettorre P, Hughes DP, editors. Sociobiology of communication: an interdisciplinary perspective. Oxford, USA: Oxford University Press; 2008. pp. 171–190.
  11. 11. Cremer S, Armitage SAO, Schmid-Hempel P. Social immunity. Curr Biol. 2007; 17(16): R693–R702. pmid:17714663
  12. 12. Wilson-Rich N, Spivak M, Fefferman NH, Starks PT. Genetic, individual, and group facilitation of disease resistance in insect societies. Annu Rev Entomol. 2009; 54: 405–423. pmid:18793100
  13. 13. Cremer S, Pull CD, Fürst MA. Social immunity: emergence and evolution of colony-level disease protection. Annu Rev Entomol. 2018; 63: 105–123. pmid:28945976
  14. 14. Lhomme P, Ayasse M, Valterová I, Lecocq T, Rasmont P. Born in an alien nest: How do social parasite male offspring escape from host aggression? PLoS One, 2012; 7(9): e43053. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043053. pmid:23028441
  15. 15. Pérez-Lachaud G, Bartolo-Reyes JC, Quiroa-Montalván CM, Cruz-López L, Lenoir A, Lachaud J-P. How to escape from the host nest: imperfect chemical mimicry in eucharitid parasitoids and exploitation of the ants’ hygienic behavior. J Insect Physiol. 2015; 75: 63–72. pmid:25770980
  16. 16. Scharf I, Pamminger T, Foitzik S. Differential response of ant colonies to intruders: attack strategies correlate with potential threat. Ethology 2011; 117(8): 731–739.
  17. 17. Diez L, Urbain L, Lejeune P, Detrain C. Emergency measures: Adaptive response to pathogen intrusion in the ant nest. Behav Processes 2015; 116: 80–86. pmid:25939763
  18. 18. Tranter C, LeFevre L, Evison SEF, Hughes WOH. Threat detection: contextual recognition and response to parasites by ants. Behav Ecol. 2015; 26(2): 396–405.
  19. 19. Vander Meer RK, Morel L. Nestmate recognition in ants. In: Vander Meer RK, Breed MD, Winston ML, Espelie KE, editors. Pheromone communication in social insects: ants, wasps, bees, and termites. Boulder, USA: Westview Press; 1998. pp. 79–103.
  20. 20. Van Zweden JS, d’Ettorre P. Nestmate recognition in social insects and the role of hydrocarbons. In: Blomquist GJ, Bagnères A-G, editors. Insect hydrocarbons: biology, biochemistry, and chemical ecology. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press; 2010. pp. 222–243.
  21. 21. Sturgis SJ, Gordon DM. Nestmate recognition in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): a review. Myrmecol News. 2012; 16: 101–110.
  22. 22. Barbero F, Bonelli S, Thomas JA, Balletto E, Schönrogge K. Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants. J Exp Biol. 2009; 212: 4084–4090. pmid:19946088
  23. 23. von Beeren C, Brückner A, Maruyama M, Burke G, Wieschollek J, Kronauer DJC. Chemical and behavioral integration of army ant-associated rove beetles—a comparison between specialists and generalists. Front Zool. 2018; 15: 8. pmid:29568316
  24. 24. Barbero F, Thomas JA, Bonelli S, Balletto E, Schönrogge K. Queen ants make distinctive sounds that are mimicked by a butterfly social parasite. Science 2009; 323(5915): 782–785. pmid:19197065
  25. 25. Di Giulio A, Maurizi E, Barbero F, Sala M, Fattorini S, Balletto E, et al. The pied piper: a parasitic beetle’s melodies modulate ant behaviors. PLoS One. 2015; 10(7): e0130541. pmid:26154266
  26. 26. Brückner A, Klompen H, Bruce AI, Hashim R, von Beeren C. Infection of army ant pupae by two new parasitoid mites (Mesostigmata: Uropodina). PeerJ. 2017; 5: e3870. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3870. pmid:29038753
  27. 27. Elliot SL, Hart AG. Density-dependent prophylactic immunity reconsidered in the light of host group living and social behavior. Ecology 2010; 91(1): 65–72. pmid:20380197
  28. 28. Simone-Finstrom M. Social immunity and the superorganism: Behavioral defenses protecting honey bee colonies from pathogens and parasites. Bee World 2017; 94(1): 21–29.
  29. 29. Diez L, Lejeune P, Detrain C. Keep the nest clean: survival advantages of corpse removal in ants. Biol Lett. 2014; 10(7): 20140306. pmid:25009241
  30. 30. Rocha FH, Lachaud J-P, Valle-Mora J, Pérez-Lachaud G. Fine individual specialization and elitism among workers of the ant Ectatomma tuberculatum for a highly specific task: intruder removal. Ethology 2014; 120(12): 1185–1195.
  31. 31. Lachaud J-P, Pérez-Lachaud G. Diversity of species and behavior of hymenopteran parasitoids of ants: a review. Psyche, 2012; Article ID 134746, 24 pp. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/134746.
  32. 32. Pérez-Lachaud G, Jervis MA, Reemer M, Lachaud J-P. An unusual, but not unexpected, evolutionary step taken by syrphid flies: the first record of true primary parasitoidism of ants by Microdontinae. Biol J Linn Soc Lond. 2014; 111(2): 462–472.
  33. 33. Lachaud J-P, Klompen H, Pérez-Lachaud G. Macrodinychus mites as parasitoids of invasive ants: an overlooked parasitic association. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 29995. pmid:27444515
  34. 34. Heraty JM. A revision of the genera of Eucharitidae (Hymenoptera: Chalcidoidea) of the world. Mem Am Entomol Inst. 2002; 68: 1–368.
  35. 35. Murray EA, Carmichael AE, Heraty JM. Ancient host shifts followed by host conservatism in a group of ant parasitoids. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013, 280: 20130495.
  36. 36. Clausen CP. The habits of the Eucharidae. Psyche 1941; 48(2–3): 57–69.
  37. 37. Clausen CP. The oviposition habits of the Eucharidae (Hymenoptera). J Wash Acad Sci. 1940; 30(12): 504–516.
  38. 38. Pérez-Lachaud G, Heraty JM, Carmichael A, Lachaud J-P. Biology and behavior of Kapala (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae) attacking Ectatomma, Gnamptogenys, and Pachycondyla (Formicidae: Ectatomminae and Ponerinae) in Chiapas, Mexico. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2006; 99(3): 567–576.
  39. 39. Wheeler WM. The polymorphism of ants, with an account of some singular abnormalities due to parasitism. Bull Am Mus Nat Hist. 1907, 23: 1–93.
  40. 40. Clausen CP. The biology of Schizaspidia tenuicornis Ashm., a eucharid parasite of Camponotus. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1923; 16(3): 195–217.
  41. 41. Ayre GL. Pseudometagea schwarzii (Ashm.) (Eucharitidae: Hymenoptera), a parasite of Lasius neoniger Emery (Formicidae: Hymenoptera). Can J Zool. 1962; 40(2): 157–164.
  42. 42. Vander Meer RK, Jouvenaz DP, Wojcik DP. Chemical mimicry in a parasitoid (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae) of fire ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). J Chem Ecol. 1989; 15(8): 2247–2261. pmid:24272384
  43. 43. Lachaud J-P, Pérez-Lachaud G, Heraty JM. Parasites associated with the ponerine ant Ectatomma tuberculatum (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): first host record for the genus Dilocantha (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae). Fla Entomol. 1998; 81: 570–574.
  44. 44. Howard RW, Pérez-Lachaud G, Lachaud J-P. Cuticular hydrocarbons of Kapala sulcifacies (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae) and its host, the ponerine ant Ectatomma ruidum (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2001; 94(5): 707–716.
  45. 45. Wilson EO, Durlach NI, Roth LM. Chemical releasers of necrophoric behavior in ants, Psyche 1958; 65: 108–114.
  46. 46. Diez L, Deneubourg J-L, Detrain C. Social prophylaxis through distant corpse removal in ants. Naturwissenschaften 2012; 99(10): 833–842. pmid:22955492
  47. 47. von Beeren C, Hashim R, Witte V. The social integration of a myrmecophilous spider does not depend exclusively on chemical mimicry. J Chem Ecol. 2012; 38(3): 262–271. pmid:22426892
  48. 48. Cook OF. The social organization and breeding habits of the cotton-protecting Kelep of Guatemala. USDA Bureau of Entomology, Technical Series N° 10; 1905. pp. 1–55.
  49. 49. Hora RR, Doums C, Poteaux C, Fénéron R, Valenzuela J, Heinze J, et al. Small queens in the ant Ectatomma tuberculatum: a new case of social parasitism. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005; 59(2): 285–292.
  50. 50. Pérez-Lachaud G, López-Méndez JA, Beugnon G, Winterton P, Lachaud J-P. High prevalence but relatively low impact of two eucharitid parasitoids attacking the Neotropical ant Ectatomma tuberculatum (Olivier). Biol Control 2010; 52(2): 131–139.
  51. 51. Dejean A, Lachaud J-P. Growth-related changes in predation behavior in incipient colonies of the ponerine ant Ectatomma tuberculatum (Olivier). Insectes Soc. 1992; 39(2): 129–143.
  52. 52. Fresneau D, Lachaud J-P. Résultats préliminaires sur l’ontogénèse d’une société d’Ectatomma tuberculatum (Hym. Formicidae). In: De Haro A, Espadaler J, editors. Processus d’acquisition précoce. Les communications. Barcelona: Publ. Unversitat Autónoma de Barcelona & Société Française pour l’Étude du Comportement Animal; 1984. pp. 437–444.
  53. 53. Champalbert A, Lachaud J-P. Existence of a sensitive period during the ontogenesis of social behavior in a primitive ant. Anim Behav. 1990; 39(5): 850–859.
  54. 54. Fénéron R. A new ethological test to study nestmate recognition in adult ants. Insectes Soc. 1996; 43(2): 131–135.
  55. 55. Howard RW, Pérez-Lachaud G. Cuticular hydrocarbons of the ectoparasitic wasp Cephalonomia hyalinipennis (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) and its alternative host, the stored product pest Caulophilus oryzae (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Arch Insect Biochem Physiol. 2002; 50(2): 75–84. pmid:12173292
  56. 56. Šobotník J, Hanus R, Jirošová A. Chemical warfare in termites. J Insect Physiol. 2010; 56(9): 1012–1021. pmid:20223240
  57. 57. Pérez-Lachaud G, Hardy ICW. Alternative hosts for bethylid parasitoids of the coffee berry borer Hypothenemus hampei (Coleoptera: Scolytidae). Biol Control 2001; 22(3): 265–277.
  58. 58. Lenoir A, d´Ettorre P, Errard C, Hefetz A. Chemical ecology and social parasitism in ants. Annu Rev Entomol. 2001; 46: 573–599. pmid:11112180
  59. 59. Diez L, Moquet L, Detrain C. Post-mortem changes in chemical profile and their influence on corpse removal in ants. J Chem Ecol. 2013; 39(11–12): 1424–1432. pmid:24242873
  60. 60. Jaffé K, Ramos C, Lagalla C, Parra L. Orientation cues used by ants. Insectes Soc. 1990; 37(2): 101–115.
  61. 61. Schatz B, Beugnon G, Lachaud J-P. Time-place learning by an invertebrate, the ant Ectatomma ruidum Roger. Anim Behav. 1994; 48(1): 236–238.
  62. 62. Franz NM, Wcislo WT. Foraging behavior in two species of Ectatomma (Formicidae: Ponerinae): individual learning of orientation and timing. J Insect Behav. 2003; 16(3): 381–410.
  63. 63. Roche-Labarbe N, Beugnon G, Lachaud J-P. Apprentissage de routes et orientation spatiale chez un insecte sur un support en trois dimensions. Actes Coll Insectes Soc. 2004; 16: 52–56.
  64. 64. Ostwald MM, Ruzi SA, Baudier KM. Ambush predation of stingless bees (Tetragonisca angustula) by the solitary-foraging ant Ectatomma tuberculatum. J Insect Behav. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10905-018-9694-9.
  65. 65. Ibarra-Núñez G, García JA, López JA, Lachaud J-P. Prey analysis in the diet of some ponerine ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and web-building spiders (Araneae) in coffee plantations in Chiapas, Mexico. Sociobiology 2001; 37(3B): 723–755.
  66. 66. Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern Applied Statistics with S. Fourth edition. New York, USA: Springer; 2002.
  67. 67. Wickham H. Reshaping data with the reshape package. J Stat Softw. 2007; 21(12): 1–20.
  68. 68. Galwey NW. Introduction to Mixed Modeling: Beyond regression and analysis of variance. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons; 2006.
  69. 69. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; 2013. URL http://www.R-project.org/.
  70. 70. Payne CM, Tillberg CV, Suarez AV. Recognition systems and biological invasions. Ann Zool Fenn. 2004; 41(6): 843–858.
  71. 71. Fénéron R, Poteaux C, Boilève M, Valenzuela J, Savarit F. Discrimination of the social parasite Ectatomma parasiticum by its host sibling species (E. tuberculatum). Psyche 2012; Article ID 134746, 11 pp. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/573541.
  72. 72. Bengston SE, Dornhaus A. Be meek or be bold? A colony-level behavioural syndrome in ants. Proc R Soc Lond B, 2014; 281: 20140508.
  73. 73. Jandt JM, Bengston S, Pinter-Wollman N, Pruitt JN, Raine NE, Dornhaus A, et al. Behavioural syndromes and social insects: personality at multiple levels. Biol Rev, 2014; 89: 48–67. pmid:23672739
  74. 74. Stroeymeyt N, Giurfa M, Franks NR. Improving decision speed, accuracy and group cohesion through early information gathering in house-hunting ants. PLoS One 2010; 5(9): e13059. pmid:20927374
  75. 75. Atkinson EB, Ellis JD. Adaptive behavior of honeybees (Apis mellifera) toward beetle invaders exhibiting various levels of colony integration. Physiol Entomol. 2011; 36(3): 282–289.
  76. 76. Halcroft M, Spooner-Hart P, Neumann P. Behavioral defense strategies of the stingless bee, Austroplebeia australis, against the small hive beetle, Aethina tumida. Insectes Soc. 2011; 58(2): 245–253.
  77. 77. Breed MD, Guzman-Novoa E, Hunt GJ. Defensive behavior of honey bees: organization, genetics, and comparisons with other bees. Annu Rev Entomol. 2004; 49: 271–298. pmid:14651465
  78. 78. von Beeren C, Maruyama M, Hashim R, Witte V. Differential host defense against multiple parasites in ants. Evol Ecol. 2011; 25(2): 259–276.
  79. 79. Parmentier T, Dekoninck W, Wenseleers T. Do well-integrated species of an inquiline community have a lower brood predation tendency? A test using red wood ant myrmecophiles. BMC Evol Biol. 2016; 16: 12. pmid:26781178
  80. 80. De la Mora A, Pérez-Lachaud G, Lachaud J-P. Mandible strike: the lethal weapon of Odontomachus opaciventris against small prey. Behav Processes 2008; 78(1): 64–75. pmid:18304760
  81. 81. Morgenstern D, King GF. The venom optimization hypothesis revisited. Toxicon 2013; 63: 120–128. pmid:23266311
  82. 82. Heraty JM, Mottern J, Peeters C. A new species of Schizaspidia, with discussion of the phylogenetic utility of immature stages for assessing relationships among eucharitid parasitoids of ants. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2015; 108(5): 865–874.
  83. 83. Torréns J, Heraty JM, Murray E, Fidalgo P. Biology and phylogenetic placement of a new species of Lasiokapala Ashmead from Argentina (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae). Syst Entomol, 2016; 41: 596–606.
  84. 84. Vander Meer RK, Wojcik DP. Chemical mimicry in the myrmecophilous beetle, Myrmecaphodius excavaticollis. Science 1982; 218(4574): 806–808. pmid:17771039
  85. 85. Krantz GW, Gómez LA, Gonzalez VE. Parasitism in the Uropodina; a case history from Colombia. In: Morales-Malacara JB, Behan-Pelletier V, Ueckermann E, Pérez TM, Estrada-Venegas EG, Badii M, editors. Acarology XI: Proceedings of the International Congress. México: Instituto de Biología and Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Sociedad Latinoamericana de Acarología; 2007. pp. 29–38.
  86. 86. Robertson PL. Pheromones involved in aggressive behavior in the ant Myrmecia gulosa. J Insect Physiol. 1971; 17(4): 691–715.
  87. 87. Dejean A. Étude comparée du comportement de prédation d´une fourmi primitive: Odontomachus troglodytes (Formicidae, Ponerinae) et de trois fourmis de la tribu des Dacetini: Strumygenys rufobrunea, Serrastruma serrula, Smithistruma emarginata (Formicidae, Myrmicinae) en présence de leur proie habituelle (termites pour les Odontomachus, collemboles pour les Dacetines). Bull SFECA. 1980; 43–59.
  88. 88. Dejean A. Étude éco-éthologique de la prédation chez les fourmis du genre Smithistruma (Formicidae, Mymmicinae, Dacetini). III. La capture des proies chez S. emarginata. Insectes Soc. 1985; 32(3): 241–256.
  89. 89. Cerdá X, Dejean A. Predation by ants on arthropods and other animals. In: Polidori C, editor. Predation in the Hymenoptera: an evolutionary perspective. Transworld Research Network. Kerala, India; 2011. pp. 39–78.
  90. 90. Akino T, Yamaoka R. Origin of oleic acid: corpse recognition signal in the ant, Formica japonica Motschlsky (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Jpn J Appl Entomol Z. 1996; 40(4): 265–271.
  91. 91. Hénaut Y, Machkour-M’Rabet S, Lachaud J-P. The role of learning in risk-avoidance strategies during spider-ant interactions. Anim Cog. 2014; 17(2): 185–195.
  92. 92. Perfecto I, Vandermeer JH. Cleptobiosis in the ant Ectatomma ruidum in Nicaragua. Insectes Soc. 1993; 40(3): 295–299.
  93. 93. Breed MD, McGlynn TP, Stocker EM, Klein AN. Thief workers and variation in nestmate recognition behavior in a ponerine ant, Ectatomma ruidum. Insectes Soc. 1999; 46(4): 327–331.
  94. 94. Richard F-J, Dejean A, Lachaud J-P. Sugary food robbing in ants: a case of temporal cleptobiosis. C R Biol, 2004; 327(5) 509–517 pmid:15255481